Female sexual preferences differ in mus spicilegus and mus musculus domesticus: the role of familiarization and sexual experience
ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 1998, 56, 1465–1470 Article No. ar980919
Female sexual preferences differ in Mus spicilegus and
Mus musculus domesticus: the role of familiarization
B. PATRIS & C. BAUDOIN
Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expe´rimentale et Compare´e, CNRS UPRES-A 7025, Universite´ Paris-Nord
(Received 19 January 1998; initial acceptance 19 February 1998;final acceptance 19 June 1998; MS. number: 5754R)ABSTRACT
Mating systems correspond to particular ecological conditions and result from proximate interactionsbetween individuals. We compared the mating preferences of female mice of two species: the housemouse, Mus musculus domesticus, and the mound-builder mouse, Mus spicilegus. Because of differences intheir habitat, we expected to observe differences in their sexual preferences. We studied femalepreferences for a familiar or an unfamiliar male and the occurrence of copulation with the unfamiliarmale, during two states of female sexual activity: (1) the postpartum oestrus of paired females, to evaluatethe stability of their sexual partnership; and (2) the oestrus of females familiarized with a male, to studythe mechanisms underlying their sexual preferences. In the polygamous house mouse, postpartumoestrous females did not show a clear preference between their familiar male and the unfamiliarone. Moreover, oestrous females, familiarized with a male (without sexual interactions), preferred anunfamiliar male and copulated with him. In contrast, postpartum oestrous females and oestrous femalesof M. spicilegus preferred their familiar male and rarely copulated with the unfamiliar male. This studyindicates a strong pair bond in established breeding pairs in M. spicilegus and shows that this bond can beestablished by familiarization, which is not the case in M. m. domesticus. Our study suggests the existenceof monogamous traits in M. spicilegus in contrast to the polygamous M. m. domesticus.
1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour
The characteristics of social organization and mating
distributed (and studied) species of mice, Mus musculus
systems result from particular ecological conditions
domesticus, the female’s choice of a male depends in
but also from the proximate interactions between indi-
viduals. In many rodent species these interactions are
partners of their species and subspecies
In M. m. domesticus, the commensal species of the
mating systems correspond to particular ecological con-
another species of mouse living under very different
Mate choice, especially in male mice, is also affected by
ecological conditions, such as the mound-builder mouse,
previous social and ontogenetic experience, mainly, but
M. spicilegus, would have a different mating system. These
not only, during the first weeks after birth
mice from the steppes of eastern Europe are mainly
characterized by their autumnal behaviour of buildingtumuli (with stocks of seeds) where they overwinter
Correspondence: B. Patris, Laboratoire d’Ethologie Expe´rimentaleet Compare´e, UPRES-A 7025 CNRS, Universite´ Paris-Nord, 93430Villetaneuse, France (email: Bruno.Patris@leec.univ-paris13.fr).
1998 The Association for the Study of Animal Behaviour1466 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 56, 6
Male–female bonding in particular should be consid-
ered as an important factor that can affect a female’schoice of a mate, as it does in several monogamousspecies (Microtus ochrogaster:
We compared the sexual preferences of female M. m.domesticus and M. spicilegus, allowed to choose betweentwo males (familiar versus unfamiliar). To evaluate thestability of their sexual partnership, we first studied
paired females in postpartum oestrus. Second, to investi-gate the mechanisms of these sexual preferences, westudied oestrous females familiarized with a male. Wethen recorded whether the female would copulate with
In the polygamous species, M. m. domesticus, we
expected the female to show interest in, and to copulatewith, the unfamiliar male. Mound-builder mice show a
high level of paternal care compared with house mice(unpublished data), which is considered to be a mono-
Figure 1. Experimental apparatus used for testing female prefer- ences. The female was placed into zone 1. Zones 2a and 3a were
areas with male odours; zones 2b and 3b, areas with active males.
Areas a and b were separated by a transparent plastic sheet with
fore, we expected the female to prefer the familiar maleand to refrain from copulating with an unfamiliar male.
16 cm and 14 cm high) for 8 days before the choice
test to make their social status similar and to stimulatetheir sexual behaviour
Animals and Breeding Conditions
We used mice derived from stocks caught in the
wild and reared in captivity for 15 generations: M. m.Choice Test Between Familiar and Unfamiliar domesticus, strain DDO (Odis, Denmark) and M. spicilegus,
strain ZYP (Pancevo, Yugoslavia). The mice were main-tained in our laboratory under a 14:10 h light:dark cycle
with lights on at 2200 hours, and an ambient temperature
The experimental apparatus consisted of a
2 C in the breeding and experimental rooms. The
mice were housed in polycarbonate cages measuring
was introduced (zone 1), connected to two other terraria
16 cm and 14 cm high, food (mice pellets, U.A.R.
type AO4) and water were supplied ad libitum, and
plastic tubes (3 cm in diameter and 9 cm in length). The
cotton was provided for nesting material. At 21 days of
acrylic opaque walls of the terraria prevented the mice
age mice of both sexes were housed in unisex sibling
seeing each other. Terraria 2 and 3 were divided in half
(zones a and b) by an acrylic transparent partition with
When they were 4–6 months old, we randomly
assigned unrelated males and females to breeding pairs ina terrarium measuring 50
each species two groups of pairs were studied: nine
We determined the receptive period of the postpartum
oestrous females behaviourally when, after delivery, we
oestrous female groups) and 10 pairs without sexual
observed the female adopt a lordotic posture after a
experience but with 15 days of familiarization (oestrous
female groups). Postpartum oestrous females were paired
was similar for the oestrous females during the daily
for several weeks with their male and these pairs had
direct interactions. At the time of the first lordotic
already had two litters. Oestrous females, during a 15-day
posture of the female after a mount we separated the dyad
period of familiarization with a male, were allowed to
and introduced the female into zone 1 of the testing
interact (except to copulate) with him daily for several
apparatus for 15 min. The familiar male and the unfam-
hours; for the rest of the day they were allowed to sniff
iliar male were placed randomly during this period into
each other by naso-nasal contact through holes in a
zones 2a and 3a which were disconnected from zone 1.
After 15 min we transferred these males into zones 2b and
Unfamiliar and unrelated males (19 M m. domesticus
3b, respectively. The tubes connecting zone 1 to zones 2a
and 19 M. spicilegus) were isolated in standard cages
and 3a were then opened and the female was allowed to
PATRIS & BAUDOIN: FEMALE MOUSE SEXUAL PREFERENCES 1467
explore freely these areas and zone 1 for 10 min. During
this period she could see the males and sniff them
through the holes of the partition during naso-nasalcontacts. Using a Psion Organizer 2 we recorded the total
duration of female presence in each zone (zone 1+tubes,
zones 2a and 3a) and the duration of sniffing familiar andunfamiliar males during naso-nasal contacts through
100 M. m. domesticus M. spicilegus M. m. domesticus M. spicilegusCopulation with Unfamiliar Male
At the end of the 10-min choice test, we closed the door
between zone 1 and zone 2a or 3a once the female had
entered the compartment of the apparatus close to the
unfamiliar male Then we removed the partition
between the female and the unfamiliar male and recorded
the presence (or absence) of copulation during 30 min. If
copulation occurred, the interaction was stopped after
the first intromission with lordosis. All the females were
then reunited with their familiar males and we recorded
M. m. domesticus M. spicilegus M. m. domesticus M. spicilegus
the first copulation as the sign that they were receptive at
the end of the experiment. We cleaned the experimentalapparatus between tests with 70% ethanol. Figure 2. (a) Time (s) spent by females in zones with odours of the familiar male () and an unfamiliar male () and (b) the time (s) spent sniffing these males: postpartum oestrous females of M. m. Data Analysis domesticus (N=9), postpartum oestrous females of M. spicilegus(N=9), oestrous females of M. m. domesticus (N=10), oestrous
females of M. spicilegus (N=10). *P<0.05; ** P<0.01; ***P<0.001.
used a paired t test (two-tailed) to compare how long thefemales stayed in the zones with odours of the familiar
the partition. If amicable interaction occurred immedi-
and unfamiliar males, and to compare how long they
ately, we left the animals together and replaced them in
sniffed familiar and unfamiliar males during naso-nasal
their cage the day after. If agonistic interactions occurred,
contacts (we verified the normality of distribution of data
we separated the animals using the central partition and
for all parameters using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test).
used the same procedure during the next few days until
Then we used an ANOVA to compare the allocation of
interactions were amicable (maximum 2 days with these
time between the two males in the two species for each
parameter, when the females were in postpartum oestrus
and in oestrus. We used only the ‘all effects’ analysis
agonistic behaviour occurred at the beginning of the
meeting (threat, pursuit, boxing); these fights lasted 1–5 s
For each species and each type of oestrus, we used the
would have separated the partners if fights were observed
McNemar test to compare the presence/absence of copu-
after 5 min but there was no persistent or injurious
lation during successive encounters with familiar and
aggression and as soon as the male perceived the female
then unfamiliar males. Then we used Fisher’s exact test
was receptive we observed only defensive behaviour by
(one-tailed) to compare the frequency of copulation with
the female refusing to copulate. This work was carried out
under licence from the French Ministry of Agriculture.
oestrous females and then for oestrous females. We used aone-tailed test since we expected a lower frequency ofcopulation with the unfamiliar male in M. spicilegusChoice Test Between Familiar and Unfamiliar Ethical Note
After their 8-day period of isolation, the unfamiliar
males behaved normally when we used them for consti-
In M. spicilegus, postpartum oestrous females spent
tuting new pairs in our breeding stock or put them back
more time near the familiar male (paired t test: t =2.59,
with their sibling brothers after a period of familiariz-
P<0.05, but did not sniff him for longer
ation. Familiarization took place in a terrarium measuring
No differences were observed for the postpartum oestrous
30 cm, divided in half with a partition between
females in M. m. domesticus. An ANOVA revealed that the
this male and his brothers, allowing olfactory communi-
time allocation between the two males differed in the two
cation and preventing aggression. After 24 h we removed
1468 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 56, 6
species and becausefemales avoid contact with unknown males during
In M. spicilegus, oestrous females spent more time in the
area close to the familiar male (paired t test: t =2.36,
Because of their differing ecology, we predicted behav-
P<0.05; and sniffed him for longer (paired t test:
ioural differences between the two species. Both oestrous
t =4.30, P<0.01; In M. m. domesticus, oestrous
and postpartum oestrous females of M. spicilegus were
females, in contrast, spent more time near the unfamiliar
more attracted to their familiar male than the unfamiliar
one (except the duration of sniffing in the postpartum
him for longer (paired t test: t =
group). Moreover, the females generally refused to copu-
An ANOVA revealed that the time allocation between the
late with an unfamiliar male. These results indicate a
two males differed in the two species (F
strong pair bond in this species between the male and
P<0.01; as well as the sniffing time allocation
postpartum female and a sexual preference induced by
familiarization in the oestrous state. Central vasopressincould act in the establishment of this sexual preference
Copulation with Unfamiliar Male
after familiarization as in the monogamous prairie voles,Microtus ochrogaster
All the females copulated with their familiar male both
at the beginning and at the end of the experiment. All the
We do not know exactly how populations of
unfamiliar males attempted to copulate with the females. M. spicilegus are organized under natural conditions.
In the postpartum oestrous state (paired females)
They build mounds structured with alternate layers of
M. spicilegus females rarely copulated with the unfamiliar
earth and seeds in the autumn after breeding
male (2/9) and preferred to copulate with their usual
partner (McNemar test: df=1, P<0.01). Four out of nine
analyses have shown that young found in the mounds
M. m. domesticus females copulated with the unfamiliar
originate from several related females and several males
male showing a preference for copulating with their
familiar male as well (McNemar test: df=1, P<0.05). Twice
ible with polygynous or polyandrous mating systems.
as may paired females copulated with the unfamiliar male
Our results concerning the existence of a pair bond
in M. m. domesticus than in M. spicilegus but the difference
between the partners could be interpreted as an indi-
was not statistically significant (one-tailed Fisher’s exact
cation of monogamy as described in several species of
In the oestrous state (familiarized females) only a few
M. spicilegus females copulated with the unfamiliar male
(unpublished) results reveal a higher paternal invest-
(3/10), and the majority preferred to copulate with their
ment by M. spicilegus males than by M. m. domesticus.
familiar male (McNemar test: df=1, P<0.01). In contrast
These results argue in favour of monogamous traits in
the majority of M. m. domesticus oestrous females copu-
lated with the unfamiliar male (8/10). Oestrous M. domes-ticus females copulated more often with an unfamiliar
male than did oestrous M. spicilegus females (one-tailed
Before concluding that M. spicilegus is monogamous,
Fisher’s exact test: P=0.037).
however, we need to demonstrate the presence of someother common characteristics in monogamous species,such as stability of this bond (long-term association),
DISCUSSION
a common territory and a symmetrical pattern ofdispersal in young males and females
Since the commensal mouse M. m. domesticus is poly-
gamous, we expected females to show sexual interest in
need to conduct field studies to determine the validity
an unfamiliar male and to copulate with him. Our results
of experimental results from the laboratory and the
partially confirm these predictions. Oestrous females
characteristics of the mating processes under natural
familiarized with a male during a 15-day period spent
more time close to the unfamiliar male than to thefamiliar one, sniffed the unfamiliar male more often, andcopulated with him. A period of familiarization was not
Acknowledgments
sufficient to induce a female sexual preference in thisspecies. Paired females in postpartum oestrus, which had
We thank Franc¸ois Bonhomme and Annie Orth for
already reproduced twice with the same male, had no
providing the two species of mice. We are grateful to
clear preference: they were not attracted to the unfamiliar
Simone Demouron for her help in taking care of the
male and did not sniff him more often. All the males
animals. We thank Jean-Luc Durand for statistical
attempted to copulate with the females and we conclude
advices, Christophe Fe´ron, Gilles Gheusi, Patrick Gouat
that their attractiveness was high. Nevertheless, about
and the anonymous referees for their comments, Andrea
half (5/9) of the females in this condition refused to
Dejean and Lesya Vynogradska for help with the English
copulate with the unfamiliar male. This could be because
in the manuscript and Lesya Vynogradska for translation
sexually inexperienced males commit infanticide in this
PATRIS & BAUDOIN: FEMALE MOUSE SEXUAL PREFERENCES 1469 References Gheusi, G., Goodall, G. & Dantzer, R. 1997. Individually distinctive
odours represent individual conspecifics in rats. Animal Behaviour,
Aldhous, P. 1989. The effects of individual cross-fostering on the 53, 935–944.
development of intrasexual kin discrimination in male laboratory
Gubernick, D. J. & Addington, R. L. 1994. The stability of
mice, Mus musculus L. Animal Behaviour, 37, 741–750.
female social and mating preferences in the monogamous
Bonhomme, F., Catalan, J., Britton-Davidian, J., Chapman, V. M.,
California mouse, Peromyscus californicus. Animal Behaviour, 47, Moriwaki, K., Nevo, E. & Thaler, L. 1984. Biochemical diversity
and evolution in the genus Mus. Biochemical Genetics, 22, Halpin, Z. T. 1980. Individual odors and individual recognition:
review and commentary. Biology of Behavior, 5, 233–248. Boursot, P., Auffray, J.-C., Britton-Davidian, J. & Bonhomme, F. Hayashi, S. 1990. Social condition influences sexual attractiveness of
1993. The evolution of house mice. Annual Review of Ecology and
dominant male mice. Zoological Science, 7, 889–894. Systematics, 291, 119–152. Hepper, P. G. 1987. The discrimination of different degrees of Bowers, J. M. & Alexander, B. K. 1967. Mice: individual recognition
relatedness in the rat: evidence for a genetic identifier. Animal
by olfactory cues. Science, 158, 1208–1210. Behaviour, 35, 549–554. Bronson, F. H. 1971. Rodent pheromones. Biology of Reproduction, Holmes, W. G. & Sherman, P. W. 1983. Kin recognition in animals. 1, 344–357. American Scientist, 71, 46–55. Bronson, F. H. 1976. Urine marking in mice: causes and effects. In: Kavaliers, M. & Colwell, D. D. 1995. Odours of parasitized males Mammalian Olfaction, Reproductive Processes and Behavior (Ed. by
induce aversive responses in female mice. Animal Behaviour, 50,
L. Doty ), pp. 119–141. New York: Academic Press. Bronson, F. H. 1979. The reproductive ecology of the house mouse. Kleiman, D. G. 1977. Monogamy in mammals. Quarterly Review of Quarterly Review of Biology, 54, 265–299. Biology, 52, 39–69. Brown, R. E. & Macdonald, D. W. 1985. Social Odours in Mammals. Kleiman, D. G. & Malcolm, J. R. 1981. The evolution of male
parental investment in mammals. In: Parental Care in MammalsCarter, C. S. & Getz, L. L. 1993. Monogamy and the prairie vole.
(Ed. by D. J. Gubernick & P. H. Klopfer), 347–387. New York:
Scientific American, 208, 70–76. Christophe, N. & Baudoin, C. 1998. Olfactory preferences in two
strains of wild mice, Mus musculus musculus and Mus musculusLenington, S. 1994. Of mice, men and the MHC. Trends in Ecology domesticus, and their hybrids. Animal Behaviour, 56, 365–369. and Evolution, 9, 455–456. Crowcroft, P. & Rowe, F. P. 1963. Social organisation and territorial Lenington, S. & Egid, K. 1989. Environmental influences on the
behaviour in the wild house mouse (Mus musculus L.). Proceedings
preferences of wild female house mice for males of differing
of the Zoological Society of London, 140, 517–531.
t-Complex genotypes. Behavior Genetics, 19, 257–265. De Catanzaro, D. & Gorzalca, B. B. 1979. Isolation-induced Lidicker, W. Z., Jr. 1976. Social structure and density regulation in
facilitation of male sexual behavior in mice. Journal of Comparative
house mice living in large enclosures. Journal of Animal Ecology, 45, Physiological Psychology, 93, 211–222. Dewsbury, D. A. 1981. An exercise in the prediction of monogamy McGill, T. E. 1962. Sexual behavior in three inbred strains of mice.
in the field from laboratory data on 42 species of muroid rodents. Behaviour, 19, 341–350. Biologist, 63, 138–162. Mandillo, S. & D’Amato, F. R. 1997. Effect of strange male odour Dobson, F. S. & Jones, W. T. 1985. Multiple causes of dispersal.
on parental care in lactating female mice. Animal Behaviour, 54, American Naturalist, 126, 855–858. Drickamer, L. C. 1992. Oestrous female house mice discriminate Mock, D. W. & Fujioka, M. 1990. Monogamy and long-term
dominant from subordinate males and sons of dominant from
pair-bonding in vertebrates. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 5,
sons of subordinate males by odour cues. Animal Behaviour, 43, Naumov, N. P. 1940. Ecology of the mound-builder mouse Mus D’Udine, B. & Alleva, E. 1983. Early experience and sexual musculus hortolanus. Works Institute Evolutionary Morphology USSR,
preferences in rodents. In: Mate Choice (Ed. by P. Bateson),
3, 33–76 (in Russian).
pp. 311–327. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Newman, K. S. & Halpin, Z. T. 1988. Individual odours and Duryadi, D. 1993. Ro
ˆle possible du comportement dans l’e´volution
mate recognition in the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster. Animal
de deux souris Mus macedonicus et Mus spicilegus en Europe
Behaviour, 36, 1779–1787.
centrale. Ph.D. thesis, Universite´ Montpellier II. Oliveras, D. & Novak, M. 1986. A comparison of paternal behaviour Egid, K. & Brown, J. L. 1989. The major histocompatibility complex
in the meadow vole Microtus pennsylvaticus, the pine vole M.
and female mating preferences in mice. Animal Behaviour, 38, pinetorum and the prairie vole M. ochrogaster. Animal Behaviour,
34, 519–526. Eisenberg, J. F. & Kleiman, D. G. 1972. Olfactory communication in Orsini, P., Bonhomme, F., Britton-Davidian, J., Croset, H.,
mammals. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 3, 10–32. Gerasimov, S. & Thaler, L. 1983. Le complexe d’espe`ces du Emlen, S. T. & Oring, L. W. 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the
genre Mus en Europe Centrale et Orientale. II Crite`res
evolution of mating systems. Science, 197, 215–233. Garza, J. C., Dallas, D., Duryadi, D., Gerasimov, S., Croset, H. & Zeitschrift fu¨r Sa¨ugetierkunde, 48, 86–95. Boursot, P. 1997. Social structure of the mound-building mouse Parmigiani, S. 1986. Rank order in pairs of communally nursing Mus spicilegus revealed by genetic analysis with microsatellites.
female mice (Mus musculus domesticus). Aggressive Behaviour, 12, Molecular Ecology, 6, 1009–1017. Gerlach, G. 1990. Dispersal mechanisms in a captive wild house Perrigo, G. & Bronson, F. H. 1982. Signalling and priming com-
mouse population (Mus domesticus Rutty). Biological Journal of the
munication: independent roles in the reproductive isolation of
Linnean Society, 41, 271–277.
spatially-separated populations of rodents. Behavioral Ecology andGetz, L. L., Carter, C. S. & Gavish, L. 1981. The mating systems Sociobiology, 10, 181–184.
of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster: field and laboratory
Pisareva, M. E. 1948. Contribution to ecology and systematic of
evidence for pair-bonding. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 8,
the mound-building mouse. Works of the Biological FacultyDniepropetrovsk State University, 32, 68–71 (in Russian). 1470 ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 56, 6 Reimer, J. D. & Petras, M. L. 1967. Breeding structure of the house Vom Saal, F. S. 1985. Time-contingent change in infanticide and
mouse Mus musculus, in a population cage. Journal of Mammalogy,
parental behavior induced by ejaculation in male mice. Physiology48, 88–99. and Behaviour, 34, 7–15. Ribble, D. O. 1991. The monogamous system of Peromyscus Winslow, J. T., Hastings, N., Carter, C. S., Harbaugh, C. R. & Insel, californicus as revealed by DNA finger-printing. Behavioral EcologyT. R. 1993. A role for central vasopressin in pair bonding in and Sociobiology, 29, 161–166.
monogamous prairie voles. Nature, 365, 545–548. Shapiro, L. E. & Dewsbury, D. A. 1986. Male dominance, female Wittenberger, J. F. & Tilson, R. L. 1980. The evolution of
choice and male copulatory behavior in two species of voles
monogamy, hypotheses and evidence. Animal Review of Ecology
(Microtus ochrogaster and Microtus montanus). Behavioral, Ecologyand Systematics, 11, 197–232. and Sociobiology, 18, 267–274. Shapiro, L. E., Austin, D., Ward, S. E. & Dewsbury, D. A. 1986. Yamazaki, K., Boyse, E. A., Mike, V., Thaler, H. T., Mathieson,
Familiarity and female mate choice in two species of voles
B. J., Abbott, J., Boyse, J., Zayas, Z. A. & Thomas, L. 1976.
(Microtus ochrogaster and Microtus montanus). Animal Behaviour,
Control of mating preferences in mice by genes in the Major
34, 90–97.
Histocompatibility Complex. Journal of Experimental Medicine, 144, Viveros, M. P. & Hernandez, R. 1989. Effects of social isolation and
crowding on sexual behaviour in the rat (Rattus norvegicus). Etologia, 1, 1–8.
a cura del dr. Antonio Del Sorbo - Specialista in Dermatologia e Venereologia antoniodelsorbo@libero.it I Cheloidi di Alibert A volte una ferita anche apparentemente banale, guarisce lasciando una cicatrice voluminosa, rossastra e soprattutto antiestetica. I cheloidi sono cicatrici abnormi che possono far seguito a intervento chirurgico (es: tiroide, mammella, etc) e questo u